nature.com

Generalization and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance in male and female rats

AbstractUnderstanding anxiety-related disorders can be advanced by studying the fear learning mechanisms implicated in the transition from adaptive to maladaptive fear. Individuals with anxiety disorders often show impaired fear extinction, pervasive avoidance, and overgeneralization of fear. While these characteristics are usually studied in isolation, their interactions are less understood. We modified the platform-mediated avoidance task to chart avoidance, generalization, and extinction in male and female rats. Male rats acquired avoidance, showed a gradient of generalization, and reduced avoidance and fear under extinction. Female rats also learned avoidance, showed gradual generalization, and extinction of defensive behaviors. Sex differences emerged in extinction learning but were subtler than expected. We present an open-source automated system for processing DeepLabCut and SimBA output to score avoidance and freezing behavior. This task effectively probes avoidance, generalization, and extinction of fear in rats, and our automated scoring approach offers a effective method to quantify defensive behaviors.

IntroductionAmong psychiatric conditions, anxiety-related disorders are the most common; a systematic review of prevalence studies over 44 countries estimated that anxiety-related disorders had a global prevalence of 7.3%1,2. A promising avenue for understanding the underpinnings of anxiety-related disorders lies in identifying fear learning mechanisms implicated in the transition from adaptive to maladaptive fear3,4,5. In particular, individuals with anxiety disorders appear to be characterized by deviations in several fear learning processes, such as extinction, avoidance, and generalization3,6,7,8. Fear conditioning in the laboratory can be used as a translational paradigm to understand the mechanisms at play in the etiology, maintenance, and treatment of anxiety-related disorders, including these three processes3,5. Indeed, after fear acquisition, during which a neutral stimulus (Conditioned Stimulus; CS) is paired with an aversive stimulus (Unconditioned Stimulus; US), additional phases can be used to study specific fear learning mechanisms in the laboratory. Avoidance can be investigated by letting specific behavioral responses prevent the occurrence of the US, generalization can be investigated by presenting a generalization stimulus (GS) that resembles the original CS and extinction can be examined by repeatedly presenting the CS without the US3. Prior human fear conditioning research has shown that people suffering from anxiety disorders show deficits in extinction learning compared with healthy controls9,10, exceedingly use avoidance strategies as a coping mechanism11,12 and exhibit increased generalization after fear learning13. To date, however, the roles of impaired extinction, pervasive avoidance, and overgeneralization in the onset or maintenance of anxiety disorders have been predominantly investigated in isolation until recently14, with research often focusing on one process at a time. Consequently, our understanding of the interplay and mutual causal influences between these three fear learning processes and their potential role in the transition from adaptive fear to clinical anxiety is limited3.In the current study, we address this gap by introducing a new version of the platform-mediated avoidance (PMA) task that allows for assessing avoidance, generalization, and extinction using a single procedure. Although Pavlovian fear conditioning is a translational paradigm that can be applied across species, our research focuses on rats for two important reasons: the possibility (1) to induce a real approach-avoidance conflict and (2) to study sex differences in a highly controlled way.A real approach-avoidance conflict is established in the PMA procedure as follows. Rats subjected to food restriction are first trained to press a lever in exchange for a food reward. In a subsequent fear acquisition phase, rats are given the opportunity to avoid a CS-signaled foot shock by stepping onto a platform during CS presentation, at the expense of being able to continue lever pressing for food, as the platform is too far from the lever. Thus, food-seeking behavior competes with fearful avoidance15. In this task, the cost of avoiding is considerable, which is a translational feature that distinguishes PMA from other active avoidance procedures16. This mimics the reality of individuals suffering from anxiety-related disorders who often face approach-avoidance dilemmas and tend to opt for avoidance strategies even when the cost of avoiding is high, a feature that is not well captured by many other lab tasks. Using the PMA task, researchers have recently investigated extinction15,17,18,19,20,21,22,23 and extinction with response prevention24,25. However, until now, the procedure has not yet been used to study generalization.As mentioned above, a second aim of our study was to investigate sex differences, in the absence of human sociocultural factors. The need for biomedical research to counter male bias is pressing. Women are twice as likely to be afflicted as men by anxiety-related disorders1. This disparity may have a biological basis that is likely to be evolutionary conserved and easier to dissect in an animal model. Remarkably, there is a reverse bias in preclinical research on anxiety, especially in neuroscience, with many more studies on male subjects than on female subjects only (5.5:1)26. In the last decade, funding agencies in Europe and North America have implemented policies to foster the inclusion of both sexes in biomedical research, and prominent journals have published editorials and commentaries providing encouragement and guidance to researchers for designing studies on sex differences and reporting on them in their publications27,28,29,30. However, an assessment of the influence of these changes in policies over a span of 10 years indicates that while there has been an increase in the inclusion of both sexes across most biological disciplines, studies often still fail to report sample size by sex or perform analyses by sex31. Even in clinical research, where formal measures from institutions fostered the increased inclusion of women in clinical trials, studies that include both sexes often fail to provide sex-specific analyses32. Ultimately, significant sex differences in etiology and symptomatology, coupled with the lack of attention to these sex differences or even the inclusion of females in research, negatively impact women’s health33.Recently, researchers have started investigating differences between male and female rodents in the PMA task. Some authors reported no sex differences in Sprague–Dawley rats34, whereas others did not report differences in acquisition but only in extinction of PMA in Long-Evans rats18. Other authors reported sex differences in acquisition and extinction in C57BL/6 J mice23. Sex differences in PMA clearly need further scrutiny.Here, we modified the rodent PMA task to include a two-day generalization phase between the ten-day avoidance acquisition phase and the four-day extinction phase. This allowed us to test avoidance, generalization, and extinction using a single procedure, and investigate if they develop differently in male and female rats. We also introduce a machine-learning approach to automate the analysis of behaviors relevant to the PMA (i.e., avoidance, freezing, and darting).We hypothesized that rats would readily acquire avoidance across two sessions of avoidance training and that males would show higher freezing than females during this training, in line with previous literature using classical fear conditioning procedures35,36,37. We further hypothesized that there would be a generalization gradient during the second generalization session, and we expected higher generalization in females than in male rats on the basis of prior contextual fear conditioning research38. Finally, we hypothesized that rats would gradually extinguish fear to the CS across the fear extinction sessions. Given results observed in other active avoidance tasks39,40, we predicted that female rats would retain higher avoidance during the extinction sessions than male rats.ResultsA clear gradient of generalization can be obtained in a modified platform-mediated avoidance task in male ratsExperiment 1 introduced a test of generalization between the acquisition and extinction of signaled platform-mediated avoidance in male rats (see Fig. 1). Rats (n = 12) were first trained to press a lever for food, after which cued avoidance was acquired. A two-day generalization test was then conducted. In the generalization session, two GSs of different frequencies were introduced: GS1 consisted of a tone of 5 kHz, moderately similar to the original CS (3 or 15 kHz, counterbalanced), whereas GS2 consisted of a tone more dissimilar to the CS (i.e., 15 or 3 kHz, depending on CS counterbalancing). The introduction of an moderately similar GS1 and a more dissimilar GS2 necessitated the counterbalanced use of two different tones as CS, to exclude that generalized responding from the CS to GS1 and GS2 would be confounded by unconditioned differences in reactivity to the tones (e.g., higher tones by default eliciting more responding than lower tones; see Methods for further details). The use of the high or low frequency tone as CS did not yield significant differences in CS responding during acquisition (see Figure S1A and Table S1).. After the generalization session, the experiment concluded with an extinction phase.Fig. 1A clear gradient of generalization can be obtained in a modified platform-mediated avoidance task in male rats (n = 12). The boxplots represent the average across the first 3 CSs on a given day, except in the generalization test where they represent the average for each stimulus type on a given day. Results are expressed in % of tone duration. (A). Graphic representation of the avoidance acquisition, generalization, and extinction phases. Different shades of blue denote different tone frequencies in generalization test. (B). Defensive behavior increased significantly over acquisition sessions: avoidance (F(2, 33) = 35.34, p < 0.001), freezing (F(2, 33) = 6.63, p = 0.004), and suppression of lever pressing (χ2(2) = 9.23, p = 0.01). (C). During generalization testing, avoidance behavior (F(2, 55) = 8.98, p < 0.001) and lever press suppression (F(2, 55) = 4.75, p = 0.012) but not freezing (F(2, 66) = 1.33, p = 0.272) differed significantly between stimuli. Avoidance was stronger for the CS than for GS2 (t(55) = 4.21, p < 0.001) and for GS1 than GS2 (t(55) = 2.55, p = 0.035). Lever press suppression was stronger during CS than GS2 (t(66) = 2.48, p = 0.041). (D). Extinction training significantly reduced avoidance (χ2(3) = 15.83, p = 0.001), freezing (χ2(3) = 9.2, p = 0.027), and suppression of lever pressing (F(3, 44) = 6.51, p < 0.001). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.Full size imageAn increase in avoidance behavior was observed in male rats over acquisition sessions (F(2, 33) = 35.34, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.66, for further statistics see Table 1), especially between day 1 and 2 (t(33) = -5.594, p < 0.001, d = -2.28) and day 2 and 10 (t(33) = -2.639, p = 0.033, d = -1.08). Similarly, freezing behavior increased over training sessions (F(2, 33) = 6.63, p = 0.004, ω2 = 0.24), both when comparing day 1 and 2 (t(33) = -3.193, p = 0.008, d = -1.30) and day 1 and 10 (t(33) = -3.111, p = 0.01, d = -1.27). Suppression of lever pressing increased over the avoidance acquisition sessions (χ2(2) = 9.23, p = 0.01, W = 0.38), between day 1 and 2 (V = 10, p = 0.021, r = -0.74) and day 1 and 10 (V = 7, p = 0.009, r = -0.82) as well. To summarize, all fear-related behaviors show similar dynamics over the avoidance acquisition sessions. Rats acquired avoidance behavior, which steadily increased over the acquisition sessions. Their freezing and suppression of lever pressing likewise increased over the first two sessions and then remained stable over subsequent avoidance acquisition days. For more details about the trial-by-trial data, see Figure S2. The use of CS of high or low frequency did not create significant differences in behaviors during acquisition (see Figure S1A).Table 1 Statistical results for Experiment 1.Full size tableDuring the two-day generalization test, there was a decrease in avoidance behavior over days (F(1, 55) = 23.03, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.09) and a significant difference in avoidance per stimulus (F(2, 55) = 8.98, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.06). More concretely, rats avoided more during CS than during GS2 presentations (t(55) = 4.21, p < 0.001, d = 0.73) and during GS1 than GS2 (t(55) = 2.55, p = 0.035, d = 0.44), whereas no differences were found between CS and GS1 (t(55) = 1.65, p = 0.233, d = 0.29). We observed a similar pattern for suppression of lever pressing, with less suppression on the second than on the first day (F(1, 55) = 17.15, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.1) and significant differences in suppression between stimuli (F(2, 55) = 4.75, p = 0.012, ωp2 = 0.03). Again, there was significantly more suppression during CS than during GS2 presentations (t(66) = 2.48, p = 0.041, d = 0.72), and no difference between CS and GS1 presentations (t(66) = 0.76, p = 0.725, d = 0.22) or GS1 and GS2 presentations (t(66) = 1.72, p = 0.207). Freezing behavior showed a slightly different picture. We observed a significant decrease in freezing from day 1 to day 2 (F(1, 66) = 48.27, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.4), but no significant differential freezing depending on stimulus type (see additional statistics in Table 1). We also calculated discrimination indices, which can be found in Figure S3 and Table S2. Overall, rats showed an orderly generalization gradient in suppression of lever pressing and avoidance. We observed clearly different responding between the CS and the more perceptually dissimilar GS2, and intermediate responding to GS1. In contrast, the generalization curve was flatter for freezing, showing similar responding to the three stimuli. We conclude that the introduction of a generalization phase between avoidance and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance was successful.After the generalization phase, four extinction sessions followed. Rats successfully extinguished avoidance (χ2(3) = 15.83, p = 0.001, W = 0.44), freezing (χ2(3) = 9.2, p = 0.027, W = 0.26) and suppression of lever pressing (F(3, 44) = 6.51, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.26) over four days of extinction. In fact, all behavioral indices were already significantly reduced by the third extinction session (avoidance: V = 66, p = 0.004, r = 1; freezing: V = 69, p = 0.016, r = 0.77; suppression of lever pressing: t(44) = 3.08, p = 0.018, d = 1.26).Assessing the modified platform-mediated avoidance task in female ratsExperiment 2 assessed whether the adapted platform-mediated avoidance task that we introduced in Experiment 1 would yield comparable results in female rats (Fig. 2).Fig. 2Assessing the modified platform-mediated avoidance task in female rats (n = 11). The boxplots represent the average across the first 3 CSs on a given day, except in the generalization test where they represent the average for each stimulus type on a given day. Results are expressed in % of tone duration. (A). Graphic representation of the avoidance acquisition, generalization, and extinction phases. Different shades of blue denote different tone frequencies in the generalization test. (B). During avoidance acquisition, an increase in avoidance (F(2, 30) = 19.64, p < 0.001) and suppression of lever pressing (χ2(2) = 11.49, p = 0.003) is observed over the avoidance acquisition sessions, but not in freezing (F(2, 30) = 1.60, p = 0.218). (C). Defensive behaviors differed significantly between stimuli over the generalization sessions: avoidance (F(2, 60) = 5.2, p = 0.008), freezing (F(2, 50) = 13.02, p < 0.001) and suppression of lever pressing (F(2, 60) = 11.84, p < 0.001). Avoidance (t(60) = 2.93, p = 0.013), freezing (t(50) = 4.84, p < 0.001) and suppression of lever pressing (t(60) = 4.39, p < 0.001) were stronger for CS than GS2. Defensive behaviors also differed between GSs; avoidance (t(60) = 2.63, p = 0.028), freezing (t(50) = 3.82, p = 0.001) and suppression of lever pressing (t(60) = 4.01, p < 0.001) were stronger for GS1 than GS2. (D). Extinction training reduced avoidance behavior (χ2(3) = 17.61, p < 0.001), freezing (χ2(3) = 21.27, p < 0.001), and suppression of lever pressing (F(3, 40) = 5.32, p = 0.003). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.Full size imageFemale rats (n = 11) readily learned to step onto the platform to avoid shock over the 10 days of acquisition (F(2, 30) = 19.64, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.53, see further statistics in Table 2). They avoided significantly more on day 2 than day 1 (t(30) = − 4.105, p = 0.008, d = -1.75) and on day 10 than day 1 (t(30) = − 6.155, p < 0.001, d = − 2.62), with no significant increase from day 2 to day 10 (see Figure S4 for trial-by-trial data). In contrast to the male rats of Experiment 1, female rats did not show a significant increase in freezing behavior over the acquisition sessions. They did show a significant increase in suppression of lever pressing over acquisition sessions (χ2 (2) = 11.49, p = 0.003, W = 0.52). This increase was not significant from the first to the second session of acquisition, but it was from the first to the tenth session (V = 0, p < 0.001, r = -1) and from the second to the tenth session (V = 6, p = 0.032, r = -0.78).Table 2 Statistical results for Experiment 2.Full size tableDuring the two-day generalization phase, female rats showed significant differences in avoidance behavior per stimulus (F(2, 60) = 5.2, p = 0.008, ωp2 = 0.11). While rats generalized their avoidance responses from the CS to GS1 (t(60) = 0.29, p = 0.953, d = 0.09), they avoided more during CS than during GS2 presentations (t(60) = 2.93, p = 0.013, d = 0.88) and during GS1 than GS2 presentations (t(60) = 2.63, p = 0.028, d = 0.79). Freezing did significantly decrease from generalization day 1 to 2 (F(1, 50) = 8.17, p = 0.006, ωp2 = 0.04), but still differed between stimuli (F(2, 50) = 13.02, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.13). Again, rats froze more to the CS than during GS2 (t(50) = 4.84, p < 0.001, d = 0.95), and more during GS1 than GS2 (t(50) = 3.82, p = 0.001, d = 0.75), but showed similar levels of freezing to CS and GS1 (t(50) = 1.02, p = 0.567). The same pattern of results was found for suppression of lever pressing. Suppression decreased significantly from generalization day 1 to 2 (F(1, 60) = 4.2, p = 0.045, ωp2 = 0.05), and differed between stimuli (F(2, 60) = 11.84, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.25). Again, rats showed more suppression during the CS than during GS2 (t(60) = 4.39, p < 0.001, d = 1.32), and more suppression during GS1 than GS2 (t(60) = 4.01, p < 0.001, d = 1.21), but showed similar suppression of lever pressing during CS and GS1 (t(60) = 0.38, p = 0.92, d = 0.12). We calculated discrimination indices, which can be found in Figure S5 and Table S3. Essentially, we observed the same generalization gradient for the three defensive behaviors. Rats showed different responding for CS and GS2 presentations in avoidance, freezing and suppression of lever pressing and intermediate responding to GS1. Therefore, we obtained a successful generalization gradient in female rats.Rats successfully extinguished avoidance (χ2(3) = 17.61, p < 0.001, W = 0.53), freezing (χ2(3) = 21.27, p < 0.001, W = 0.64) and suppression of lever pressing (F(3, 40) = 5.32, p = 0.003, ω2 = 0.23) over the course of the four extinction sessions. As in Experiment 1, avoidance and freezing were significantly decreased by the third session (avoidance: V = 66, p < 0.001, r = 1; freezing: V = 61, p = 0.01, r = 0.85). Suppression of lever pressing only reached statistically significant extinction by the fourth session (t(44) = 3.75, p = 0.003, d = 1.6).For exploratory purposes, we compared the results in male rats (Experiment 1) to those obtained in female rats (Experiment 2). To summarize, when comparing both experiments, we found that male rats showed higher freezing during acquisition than female rats (F(1, 21) = 20.81, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.27). Moreover, during generalization, interactions between sex and day were observed in avoidance (F(1, 105) = 6.83, p = 0.01, ωp2 = 0.01) and freezing (F(1, 105) = 32.02, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.08). We observed that males showed a significant reduction of avoidance from generalization day 1 to 2 (V = 548, p < 0.001, r = 0.74), while this decrease in avoidance was not significant in females (V = 309, p = 0.405, r = 0.17). In contrast, both males (V = 666, p < 0.001, r = 0.54) and females (V = 433, p = 0.005, r = 0.54) showed a significant decrease in freezing over generalization tests. In suppression of lever pressing, we detected an interaction of sex and stimulus (F(2, 105) = 5.07, p = 0.008, ωp2 = 0.03). While females suppressed lever pressing differently depending on the stimulus presented (χ2(2) = 16.25, p < 0.001, Ε2R = 0.25), males did not (χ2(2) = 4.03, p = 0.133, Ε2R = 0.06). Detailed results can be found in Table S4 and Figure S6,7. To evaluate these apparent sex differences in the acquisition and generalization of platform-mediated avoidance, we conducted Experiment 3 in order to systematically evaluate these potential sex differences.Subtle sex differences in the modified version of the platform-mediated avoidance taskHere, we set out to replicate and confirm the sex differences observed during acquisition and generalization sessions in avoidance, freezing and suppression in the modified platform-mediated avoidance task that we observed across the previous two experiments, testing preregistered hypotheses derived from those preceding experiments, while also taking into account the females’ estrous stage (Fig. 3).Fig. 3Subtle sex differences in the modified version of the platform-mediated avoidance task (n = 27). The boxplots represent the average across the first 3 CSs on a given day, except in the generalization test where they represent the average for each stimulus type on a given day. Results are expressed in % of tone duration. (A). Graphic representation of the avoidance acquisition, generalization, and extinction phases. Different shades of blue denote different tone frequencies in the generalization test. (B). Defensive behavior increased significantly over acquisition sessions in both sexes: avoidance (F(2, 75) = 114.21, p < 0.001), freezing (F(2, 50) = 8.63, p < 0.001) and suppression of lever pressing (F(2, 50) = 18.52, p < 0.001). (C). Both sexes showed a comparable decrease in avoidance behavior between generalization sessions 1 and 2 (F(1, 125) = 10.85, p = 0.001) and differences in avoidance between stimuli (F(2,125) = 51.18, p < 0.001). Rats avoided significantly more during the CS than during GS2 (V = 898, p = 0.004) and avoided more during GS1 than GS2 (V = 963, p < 0.001). We observed a decrease in freezing from generalization day 1 to 2 (F(1, 125) = 55.27, p < 0.001) and significant differences in freezing between stimuli (F(2,125) = 19.13, p < 0.001). For freezing, all pairwise comparisons between CS, GS1 and GS2 were significant (p < 0.001). For suppression of lever pressing during generalization, we found a significant interaction between day and stimulus (F(2, 125) = 4.34, p = 0.015), with specifically suppression of lever pressing during GS2 decreasing from session 1 to session 2 (χ2(1) = 7.61, p = 0.006). (D). Extinction training significantly reduced avoidance (F(3,75) = 22.19, p < 0.001) and suppression of lever pressing (F(3,100) = 16.75, p < 0.001). We found an interaction between sex and day in freezing behavior (F(3, 75) = 4.41, p = 0.006). Specifically, males froze more than females on day 3 (W = 134, p = 0.037). Female rats showed significantly more suppression than male rats (F(1, 100) = 4.44, p = 0.038). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.Full size imageMale (n = 14) and female (n = 13) rats (total n = 27) successfully and similarly acquired avoidance behavior over the 10-day acquisition period (F(2, 75) = 114.21, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.74, see Table 3 for complete statistics). Avoidance behavior increased significantly from the first to the second session of avoidance acquisition (t(75) = -11.35, p < 0.001, d = -3.09) and was maintained over the course of training (Day 1–10: t(75) = -14.31, p < 0.001, d = -3.9; Day 2–10: t(75) = -2.96, p = 0.011, d = -0.81). Freezing levels (F(2, 50) = 8.63, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.12) and suppression of lever pressing (F(2, 50) = 18.52, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.22) increased across training sessions and did not differ between sexes either. However, a significant interaction was observed between the different CSs used (high or low frequency tones), sex and day (see Figure S1C and Table S1).Table 3 Statistical results for Experiment 3.Full size tableWe observed no sex differences in avoidance, freezing, or suppression of lever pressing during the generalization sessions either. There was a reduction in avoidance between both sessions (F(1, 125) = 0.11, p = 0.001, ωp2 = 0.02) and a generalization gradient in the expected direction (F(2, 125) = 51.18, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.19). Specifically, no significant differences were present when comparing avoidance behavior during CS and GS1 (V = 893, p = 0.196, r = 0.2), but avoidance differed between CS and GS2 (V = 898, p = 0.004, r = 0.47) and between GS1 and GS2 (V = 963, p < 0.001, r = 0.57). Similarly, we observed a reduction in freezing from generalization session 1 to 2 (F(1, 125) = 55.27, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.08) and pairwise differences in freezing between all stimuli (F(2, 125) = 19.13, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.05; CS-GS1: V = 1258, p < 0.001, r = 0.69; CS-GS2: V = 1226, p < 0.001, r = 0.65; GS1 – GS2: V = 1250, p < 0.001, r = 0.68), meaning that rats showed different freezing behavior for the different tones. For suppression of lever pressing, we found a significant interaction between day and stimulus type (F(2, 125) = 4.34, p = 0.015, ωp2 = -0.01), with a reduction in suppression of lever pressing between both generalization sessions for GS2 only (χ2(1) = 7.61, p = 0.006, Ε2R = 0.14). In addition, we evaluated discrimination indices of avoidance and freezing, which can be found in Figure S8 and Table S5.Four extinction sessions followed the generalization phase. Avoidance behavior extinguished significantly over the extinction sessions (F(3, 75) = 22.19, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.15), with a significant decrease already from the first to the second extinction session (V = 277, p = 0.002, r = 0.7), without any sex differences. We observed a significant interaction between sex and day for freezing during the extinction sessions (F(3, 75) = 4.41, p = 0.006, ωp2 = 0.05). Looking at simple effects of day per sex, we found that both females (χ2(3) = 9.94, p = 0.019, Ε2R = 0.19) and males (χ2(3) = 24.17, p < 0.001, Ε2R = 0.44) showed a reduction in freezing behavior over sessions. Concretely, we found that in day 3, males showed higher freezing than their female counterparts (W = 134, p = 0.037, r = 0.47). We also found a reduction in suppression of lever pressing over extinction sessions (F(3,100) = 16.75, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.3). Males showed significantly lower suppression of lever pressing than females (F(1, 100) = 4.44, p = 0.038, ω p2 = 0.03). A significant decrease in suppression of lever pressing was apparent from the third session onwards (t(100) = 4.53, p < 0.001, d = 1.23).Using DeepLabCut and SimBA, we processed our videos to obtain speed traces of each rat (see Methods and OSF for further information). We hypothesized that 40% of female rats would be classified as darters according to previous research 41. However, in our case, 100% (13/13) of female rats were classified as darters and 86% (12/14) of male rats were also classified as darters. Due to the small numbers of non-darters, it is not possible to make reliable comparisons between darters and non-darters. Using a Chi-square test, we investigated whether there was a higher likelihood that females were classified as darters compared to males. However, the results did not support this exploratory hypothesis (χ2(1) = 0.46, p = 0.496).Additionally, we investigated whether there were differences in the number of darting bouts between male and female darters during avoidance acquisition, generalization testing and extinction training using a negative binomial regression model (see Figure S9). The model used to investigate differences in number of darts in avoidance acquisition depending on sex and day was not significant overall (χ2(3) = 5.93, p = 0.115), suggesting that neither day (β = 0.006, SE = 0.017, p = 0.710) nor sex (β = -0.239, SE = 0.206, p = 0.246) were significant predictors of darting. Similarly, the model used to investigate differences on darts during the generalization phase depending on sex and day was not significant overall (χ2(3) = 3.64, p = 0.303) nor in extinction (χ2(3) = 6.74, p = 0.08) (see coefficients in Table S6).We assessed the estrous stage of the female rats for 10 days after the last extinction session and inferred estrous stage during training retrospectively to prevent a negative impact of the estrous staging procedure on our behavioral results. We divided the four stages of the estrous cycle into high estrogen phase (i.e., proestrus and estrus) and low estrogen phase (i.e., metestrus and diestrus)42. This categorization was determined by the estrous stage observed on the last day of extinction training. We then tested whether there were differences between the estrous groups for the three behavioral outcomes (avoidance, freezing, and suppression of lever pressing) on the last extinction day (see Figure S10). We did not find significant differences in avoidance (t(11) = 1.37, p = 0.198, d = 0.76), freezing (t(11) = 1.16, p = 0.271, d = 0.64) or suppression of lever pressing (t(11) = 1.27, p = 0.23, d = 0.68).Persistence of avoidanceIn prior research using the platform-mediated avoidance task, persistent avoidance was reported in approximately 25% of (male) rats despite extinction17 or extinction with response prevention25. Here, we used a preregistered criterion to classify a rat as a persistent avoider if on the first block of the first extinction day (i.e., after two sessions of generalization) its time on the platform during the CS presentation was more than 50%.According to this definition, in Experiment 1, we found that 5/12 rats (42%) qualified as persistent avoiders (see Figure S11A). We analyzed differences between persistent avoiders and non-persistent avoiders in freezing and suppression of lever pressing on the last day of avoidance acquisition, but we did not find any significant differences between groups (see Table S7 for all statistics). We also assessed differences in freezing and suppression of lever pressing between groups during the first extinction session. We did not find significant differences in freezing behavior, but we did find that persistent avoiders showed higher suppression of lever pressing than non-persistent avoiders (t(10) = -2.3, p = 0.044, d = -1.46).In Experiment 2, 5 rats (5/11, 45%) met our predefined criterion for persistent avoidance (see Figure S11B). We conducted the same analyses as described for Experiment 1 and found no differences between the subgroups on the last avoidance acquisition session, nor on the first extinction day (see Table S7 for all statistics).In Experiment 3, 15 rats (15/27, 56%) met our preregistered criterion for persistent avoidance (see Fig. 11C). We found no differences between persistent and non-persistent avoiders in terms of freezing or suppression of lever pressing on the last day of avoidance acquisition (see Table S7). Freezing in the subgroups did not differ on the first day of extinction either. However, we did find a significant difference between persistent and non-persistent avoiders in suppression of lever pressing (F(1, 23) = 16.31, p < 0.001, ωp2 = 0.29), which is not surprising given that rats that avoided a lot during this first extinction day were classified as persistent avoiders, and avoidance implies that the rat is not able to press the lever. If we were to consider the first block of the third extinction session instead, as we have done previously22, we would identify 6 persistent avoiders (6/27, 22.22%) which is consistent with previous literature17,25. On extinction day 1, we then find significantly more freezing (F(1, 23) = 8.49, p = 0.008, ωp2 = 0.22) and suppression of lever pressing (F(1, 23) = 5.87, p = 0.024, ωp2 = 0.09) in persistent avoiders than in non-persistent avoiders.Additionally, we explored whether persistent avoiders showed higher rates of generalization. We compared discrimination indices for avoidance and freezing between persistent and non-persistent avoiders in the three experiments (see Table S8). In Experiment 1, we did not find differences in discrimination between persistent and non-persistent avoiders. However, in Experiment 2, we did find that persistent avoiders exhibited lower discrimination in avoidance behavior than non-persistent avoiders overall (F(1, 9) = 16.19, p = 0.003, ωp2 = 0.26). In Experiment 3, we did not find any significant differences between persistent or non-persistent avoiders again.DiscussionHere, we showed how avoidance, generalization, and extinction can be investigated in male and female rats, using a new, integrated platform-mediated avoidance task. Moreover, we developed scripts to facilitate the adoption of machine learning tools to analyze the animals’ behavior.Building on the predominantly male-based prior work using this task, we initially tested the introduction of a generalization phase between avoidance learning and extinction training in the platform-mediated avoidance (PMA) task in male rats only (Experiment 1) and subsequently validated the modified PMA task in female rats (Experiment 2). Indeed, at the time of designing these experiments, only one published study34 had tested female rats in the PMA task, without extinction session. Thus, given the lack of studies applying the PMA in females at that point, it was crucial to determine that we would be able to produce valid results when using similar parameters in females as we had used for males in Experiment 1. An exploratory comparison of Experiments 1 and 2 suggested potential, albeit subtle, sex differences in avoidance, generalization, and extinction. In a confirmatory test, we conducted Experiment 3, where we included both sexes, to assess the reliability of those observations.AvoidanceAvoidance acquisition was successful in all three experiments, comparable to previous studies15, particularly in the avoidance measurement and supported by freezing and suppression of lever pressing during the acquisition of avoidance. Females in Experiment 2 did not show an increase of freezing over acquisition sessions, unlike male rats in Experiment 1. This difference was not present in Experiment 3 where rats of both sexes showed similar freezing levels.GeneralizationThe generalization test phase inserted between avoidance acquisition and extinction yielded an orderly generalization gradient, where we observed markedly different responding between the CS and the more perceptually dissimilar GS2, with GS1 yielding intermediate responding. In Experiment 1, this generalization gradient was present for avoidance and suppression of lever pressing, while male rats showed similar levels of freezing across all stimuli. In contrast, in Experiment 2, females presented a generalization gradient across all measured behavioral outcomes. When comparing Experiment 1 and 2 we detected some sex differences. For instance, in males there was a reduction of avoidance behavior between generalization day 1 and 2 but this reduction in responding was not significant in females. In Experiment 3, where male and female rats were tested simultaneously, we did not find any sex differences in generalization.ExtinctionAfter generalization testing, we performed extinction training. In Experiment 1 and 2 we observed a successful reduction of all fear-related behaviors, similar to other published studies15. Comparing both experiments, we did not observe any sex differences. Surprisingly, in Experiment 3 we did observe a significant interaction between sex and day for freezing over extinction. However, the effect was subtle as the only significant difference between both sexes was found on day 3. Additionally, females showed higher suppression of lever pressing than male rats.Sex differencesAcross the presented studies, we observed minor sex differences only. The differences were more pronounced when comparing Experiments 1 and 2 than in Experiment 3, where subtle sex differences were observed in extinction only. The lack of expected sex differences in the third experiment, e.g., in generalization, may be explained by the increased experimental control afforded by testing both sexes concurrently. Additionally, Experiment 1 was performed by a male researcher and Experiment 2 by a female researcher, while in Experiment 3 both sexes were tested by one and the same female researcher (different from the researcher testing animals in Experiment 2). It has been reported previously in the literature that the sex of the experimenter can influence the measured outcomes of neuroscience experiments43,44. Additionally, in Experiment 3 the 2-s US co-terminated with the 30-s CS during avoidance acquisition, whereas in Experiments 1 and 2 the 2-s US started at the offset of the 30-s CS. We cannot strictly rule out that behavioral differences could be due to this subtle difference in CS-US delay between experiments.The absence of notable sex differences, even with the potential confounding factor of food motivation45, is noteworthy. This suggests that the modified PMA protocol can be used to examine various manipulations and their impacts on avoidance, generalization, and extinction, with the assumption that any sex differences in a control condition will be subtle.In adult rats, there have been no reported sex differences in acquisition of platform-mediated avoidance in Sprague–Dawley34 or in Long-Evans18. However, the study in Long-Evans rats reported sex differences in extinction. They reported that male rats extinguished avoidance, but female rats did not exhibit a significant reduction in time spent on the platform over the extinction sessions. Additionally, they found that female rats showed higher freezing levels and less lever pressing during extinction. These results contradict our own. However, there are some differences to consider between both studies. First, we used a different rat strain. Second, Landin and Chandler17 report a total number of lever pressing and lever pressing during tone presentation, whereas we report suppression of lever pressing. While these measures are related to each other, females could show less lever pressing but equal suppression of lever pressing to their male counterparts and vice versa. Lastly, their study design has some differences from ours. They included an avoidance test (but no generalization test) after acquisition, five sessions of reconditioning, and then five days of extinction training, which consisted of 15 CS presentations.A recent study reported significant sex differences between male and female mice23 in the PMA task. They showed that, after the 3rd day of avoidance training, 48% of female mice were non-avoiders (receiving 3 or more shocks on the third training day) and that female avoiders and non-avoiders did not differ in their freezing during acquisition. After the third day, they performed an extinction training and a test session. They observed that females showed persistent avoidance after extinction, while males did not. These results are in contradiction to ours, but several important differences between both studies should be noted. Not only did they use mice, but their study left out the lever pressing component of the PMA task. While this may control for differences in food-seeking behavior between sexes45, it removes the approach-avoidance conflict that makes the PMA procedure such a relevant translational tool to model clinical avoidance.Sex differences have been reported in avoidance, generalization or extinction using other tasks than PMA. Studies using different active avoidance paradigms have reported that females acquire avoidance faster than males39,46. There have also been reports of sex differences in generalization testing. Females showed good discrimination after limited training, while males did not. However, after extended training, female rats began to generalize their fear responses due to impaired safety learning while male rats showed significant discrimination47. Another study found that females in low-estrogen phases of their estrous cycle showed discrimination, similar to males, while females in high-estrogen phases showed generalization between two different cues48. There is evidence suggesting that estrogen could be mediating effects of contextual fear generalization in a passive avoidance task38. Regarding extinction of avoidance responding, previous research reported that while both sexes showed similar extinction of avoidance, females showed stronger reductions in freezing than males37. There is more evidence of sex differences in fear extinction, in humans and in rodents, from hormonal effects49,50 to neurobiological effects51,52,53 (see review54). However, results in the literature are mixed and some authors did not observe sex differences in extinction in rats55 or humans56.We investigated darting behavior in our third experiment, as previous research suggested that darting may be a conditioned behavior expressed largely by female rats41,57, although further work challenged this finding in mice and rats58,59. Surprisingly, in our experiment we observed that all females were darters and 86% of male rats were considered darters as well. This was surprising because it was previously reported that the number of darters is around 40% in female rats and 10% in male rats41. The differences observed in this study compared to previous research may be due to previous research using Pavlovian fear conditioning only. In those experiments, darting is primarily an escape response. In contrast, in the present experiment, darting can serve either as a flight response towards a safe location (i.e., platform) or as an approach response to a reward (i.e., lever). Nevertheless, a previous experiment using PMA in mice found darters neither in males nor females23.Persistent avoidanceHere, we used a preregistered criterion to classify a rat as a persistent avoider if on the first block of the first extinction day (i.e., after two sessions of generalization testing) its time on the platform during the CS presentation was more than 50%. While we were able to detect persistent avoiders (Experiment 1: 42%, Experiment 2: 45% and Experiment 3: 56%), we did not find consistent differences between persistent avoiders and non-persistent avoiders as reported in other studies17,25. This could have several reasons: (1) the introduction of a generalization phase in which the CS was partially reinforced, or (2) our use of a criterion to classify persistent avoiders that was not stringent enough to select true persistent avoider rats. To address the last possibility, we reanalyzed the data by applying criteria used in previous studies17,25, but using those, we did not detect any persistent avoiders in Experiments 1 and 2. In Experiment 3, we found only 2 persistent avoiders (2/30: 7%), which is lower than the percentage found in those published studies and a value too small to permit robust analyses. If we considered the first block of the third extinction session instead in Experiment 3, as we have done previously22, we find 6 persistent avoiders (6/27, 22.22%) which is consistent with previous literature17,25. We found that these persistent avoiders showed higher freezing and suppression of lever pressing than non-persistent avoiders on extinction day 1. However, these differences were not present at the end of acquisition as has been reported elsewhere25.In addition, we explored whether persistent avoiders would show lower discrimination in avoidance and freezing during the generalization phase than non-persistent avoiders. We only found indications for this in Experiment 2, where only female rats were tested (and in avoidance behavior only). Again, the lack of significant findings might be related to the criteria that we preregistered to classify persistent avoiders or to a lack of power for such subgroup comparisons. Further research regarding the potential relationship between persistent avoidance and generalization is needed.Open-source automated behavioral scoring in the PMAWe present a series of R scripts to facilitate the processing of outputs obtained from DeepLabCut and SimBA. The combined use of DeepLabCut and SimBA for the assessment of defensive behavior has already been validated by other authors60. These scripts are specifically adapted to analyze avoidance, freezing, and darting in the (modified) PMA task. Minimal coding knowledge is required, and we have provided explanations on OSF to help customize this solution to other interested users. These R scripts are readily adaptable to PMA tasks without generalization phase. The scripts are written to facilitate analysis of behaviors pre tone, during tone and post tone presentation. The use of supervised machine learning for the freezing analysis instead of purely pixel movement or motion indices61, and our high ICC results for avoidance and freezing between a human scorer and the automated results, raise our confidence in this automated solution. Finally, sharing behavioral classifiers, analysis pipelines, and datasets obtained using such tools across laboratories could foster reproducibility in behavioral neuroscience62.ConclusionsIn summary, this study presented a comprehensive behavioral procedure that can be used to study avoidance, generalization, and extinction in male and female rats. In addition, we provided a protocol including scripts to automatically analyze relevant behaviors in the platform-mediated avoidance setup using open-source tools.MethodsPreregistration and data availabilityThe first experiment reported here, conducted in males only, was not preregistered. For the other two experiments, the design, procedures, sample sizes, and analysis plans were registered on the Open Science Framework (OSF) before the start of data collection. Data and scripts for the three experiments can be found on OSF as well: https://osf.io/ncb8j/.SubjectsAll experiments were performed in accordance with Belgian and European laws (Belgian Royal Decree of 29/05/2013 and European Directive 2010/63/EU) and the ARRIVE 2.063 guidelines and approved by the KU Leuven animal ethics committee (project license number: 011/2019). The number of animals used was calculated based on previous studies. The first study was conducted in 8-week-old male Sprague–Dawley rats (270–300 g at arrival, Janvier Labs, Le Genest‐Saint‐Isle, France), the second study was conducted in 8-week-old female Sprague–Dawley rats (200–240 g at arrival, Janvier Labs) and the third study conducted in 8-week-old male (270–300 g at arrival) and female (200–240 g at arrival) rats (Janvier Labs). Animals were housed in groups of 3 on a 12-h light–dark cycle (lights on at 7 am), and experiments were performed between 9 am and 4 pm. The cages had bedding and cage enrichment in the form of a red polycarbonate tunnel hanging from the top of the grid. Water was available ad libitum for the entire experiment, except during behavioral testing. Food was provided ad libitum until 1 day before the start of the experiments. From then on, animals were fed ad libitum for an hour after each test session. Animals were habituated to handling for 2 days before the start of the experiment.ApparatusSix identical operant chambers (30.5 cm width, 25.4 cm depth, and 30.5 cm height; Rat Test Cage, Coulbourn Instruments, Pennsylvania, USA) were used simultaneously and were enclosed in sound-attenuating boxes. A 12.8 cm by 15.2 cm platform made of a non-transparent red plastic 3-mm sheet covered approximately ¼ of the grid floor and was always placed in the corner opposite to the lever. Experiments were conducted using Graphic State 3 (Coulbourn Instruments, Pennsylvania, USA) with the house light on. Behavior was continuously recorded during experimental sessions with an IP (Internet Protocol) camera (Foscam C1, Shenzhen, China).Lever press trainingLever press training consisted of 1 h of training for 10 consecutive days in Experiments 1 and 2. In Experiment 3, lever press training consisted of 30 min of training for 10 consecutive days. Rats were gradually shaped to lever press for grain-based pellets (Experiment 1 and 2: 45 mg 5TUM, TestDiet, St. Louis, MO, USA; Experiment 3: Grain-Based Dustless Precision Pellets® for Rodent, Bio-Serv, Frenchtown, NJ, USA) on a variable interval schedule of reinforcement averaging 30 s (VI-30 s). Rats that did not reach the criterion of an average of 10 lever presses per minute across the final VI-30 s session were excluded from further analyses (this resulted in the exclusion of three animals in Experiment 3). For Experiment 2, we initially preregistered a criterion of 15 lever presses per minute; however, we lowered this criterion after lever press training to avoid excluding too many rats and in line with the criterion of 10 lever presses per minute in other recent reports using the PMA task17.The first training phase consisted of one session of magazine training with the lever retracted, where food pellets were delivered at fixed 2-min intervals. The second phase of lever press training consisted of magazine training with the lever extended, where food pellets were delivered at fixed 2-min intervals, supplemented with direct delivery of a food pellet upon each lever press. This phase lasted one day minimally and required at least 1 lever press/min on average to pass on to the next phase. If there were no lever presses during the first half of the third day of this phase of training, hand shaping was performed. The next phases consisted of lever press training on increasing variable ratio (VR) schedules, progressing from VR 3 through VR 5 and VR 15 to VR 30, where pellets were delivered after a variable number of lever presses, with an increasing average reinforcement criterion per session. On VR 3, a criterion of at least 3 lever press/min was set to pass to the next schedule, a criterion of at least 5 lever press/min for VR 5, a criterion of at least 10 lever press/min for VR 15, and a criterion of at least 15 lever press/min for VR 30. The last phase consisted of training on a VI 30 schedule where the first lever press after a variable refractory period averaging 30 s yielded pellet delivery.Experiment 1Male rats (n = 12) underwent testing in a study that consisted of three phases: avoidance acquisition, generalization testing, and extinction training.Avoidance acquisition lasted for 10 days in accordance with previous research15,17,19,64. On each day, 9 30-s CSs were presented, with an ITI (intertrial interval) averaging 180 s (150 s to 210 s). This ITI was also used in all subsequent sessions. The CS consisted of a pure 3-kHz tone for half of the animals and a 15-kHz tone for the others (counterbalanced). Upon CS termination, a 2-s, 0.4-mA foot shock US was delivered through the grid floor. If the rats would be on the platform during US presentation, they would not experience the foot shock.After the 10 days of avoidance acquisition, a two-day generalization test followed. Each generalization session consisted of 3 blocks of 3 trials. Each block started with the original CS and then 2 GSs were presented (GS1: 7 kHz, GS2: 15 kHz, or 3 kHz, depending on CS counterbalancing). The order of the GSs was counterbalanced between animals and test sessions. Each tone lasted for 30 s and no US followed these tone presentations. At the end of each generalization test session, we presented a reminder CS-US pairing to minimize extinction learning. That additional CS is not included in the analyses presented here.Finally, the experiment concluded with four daily extinction training sessions. Each extinction session consisted of the original 30-s CS being presented nine times, as during the avoidance acquisition session but without US delivery.Experiment 2Female rats (n = 11) were tested in the same procedure as described above. Note that rats were handled by a different researcher for this study, and the two experiments were not conducted in parallel. Other than that, the experimental setup and conditions, the experimental design, and procedures were the same.Experiment 3The experimental design closely mimicked that of Experiments 1 and 2, with a few specific exceptions. First, males and females (n = 27, as 3 rats were excluded as they did not reach lever pressing learning criterion) were tested simultaneously and by the same experimenter. Second, during avoidance training the US co-terminated with the CS rather than occurring at the offset of the CS.Estrous cycle stagingEstrous staging was performed daily for 10 consecutive days after the fourth extinction training session of Experiment 3, to retroactively estimate the estrous stages throughout the behavioral tests without performing the somewhat stressful procedure at the time of these tests. The procedure was performed as described in Marcondes et al., 200265. In short, rats were brought into a quiet room, different from the experimental test room, where a vaginal smear was collected. Saline (100 µl) was introduced at the entrance of the vaginal canal to flush vaginal cells. The flushed solution was placed on a glass slide and protected with a coverslip for subsequent examination under a light microscope.BehaviorAvoidance and freezing during CS presentations were scored manually from the videos. The experimenter was blinded to the test session day and group to the extent possible (shock delivery could give away avoidance training session). Freezing and avoidance were scored as a percentage of CS duration. Avoidance was considered present when at least two paws and the animal’s center of mass were on the platform. Freezing was defined as full immobility except for minimal movements associated with breathing.A discrimination index of avoidance and freezing during generalization was established by calculating the difference between freezing or avoidance during the presentation of a given tone (i.e., CS) and another tone (i.e., GS1), divided by the sum of freezing or avoidance to both tones66. This was done for all the different combinations of CS and GSs.Lever presses were recorded using Graphic State 3 software (Coulbourn Instruments, Pennsylvania, USA). Lever presses produced 1 min before the tone (pretone) and during the tone were aggregated per block (each block consists of 3 tones), after which the following formula was applied to calculate suppression of lever pressing: (pretone rate—tone rate)/(pretone rate + tone rate) × 10067. Resulting values range from no suppression of lever pressing (0%) to full suppression of lever pressing (100%). For blocks where pretone and tone values were both 0, the rat’s suppression was substituted by the mean group average for that block.We analyzed darting behavior in Experiment 3 using DeepLabCut and SimBA to calculate the speed in cm/s for each animal. We considered a discrete darting bout when an animal moves with a minimum velocity of 23.5 cm/s during any CS presentation. However, an animal is considered a darter if it exhibits at least one dart between the 3rd CS-US pairing and the 7th CS-US pairing of the first avoidance training session41.DeepLabCutIn addition to the manual scoring of behavior, we also used DeepLabCut (version 2.3.9) for body part tracking in Experiment 368,69. We labeled 200 frames taken from 10 videos, each video coming from a different animal; 95% of these frames were used for training. Five body parts were selected: nose, left ear, right ear, centroid, and tail base. We used a RestNet-50 neural network70,71 with default parameters for 300,000 training iterations. We validated with 1 shuffle and found that the test error was 3.82 pixels, and the training error was 1.54 pixels (image size: 455 × 256 pixels, downsampled from original 1280 × 720 pixels). This network was used to reanalyze all videos from Experiment 3.SimBATo further analyze pose estimation data obtained with DeepLabCut, we used SimBA (version 1.96.1)62. We created a single animal project configuration with user-defined body parts. We defined pixels per mm by considering the distance between the width of the bottom of the fear conditioning box.To analyze avoidance behavior, we defined the platform and adjacent wall sections as the region of interest (ROI). If the centroid body part was inside the ROI, this was considered as avoidance behavior. Similarly, to analyze darting behavior, we used the analyze distances/velocities with 1 s time-bin modules (see further details on OSF).We constructed a random forest classifier in SimBA to assess freezing. Four videos were manually annotated; 25,876 frames (862.53 s) were annotated with freezing present and 58,259 frames (1941.97 s) with freezing absent. The start frame was defined as the first frame in which the animal stopped moving, and the end frame was defined as the last frame before the animal started moving again. We built the model with the following settings: n_estimators = 2000, RF_criterion = gini, RF_max_features = sqrt, RF_min_sample_leaf = 1, under_sample_ratio = 2, class_weights = custom: Freezing present = 2; Freezing Absent = 1, with an 80% training split. The evaluation performance of the freezing classifier had an F1 score of 0.85, precision of 0.83, and recall of 0.87 for freezing present and F1 score of 0.92, precision of 0.93, and recall of 0.91 for freezing absent. We set the discrimination threshold at Pr = 0.65 and a minimum duration of 500 ms.After that, we performed the cue light analysis add-on from SimBA. In our setup, a dim cue light is on for 2 s at the start of any given tone presentation. Using this add-on, we obtain a file with all instances when our light is on. We further processed those data using our own R scripts (see OSF for detailed instructions) to detect when our tone presentations occurred and to obtain freezing and avoidance behavior during CS presentations only.A subset of videos from SimBA was randomly selected and compared with manual scoring for freezing and avoidance. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was calculated by comparing the scoring of 9 tone presentations over 10 randomly selected videos between human scorers and the SimBA results. The ICC for the avoidance behavior was 0.996 95% CI [0.994, 0.997] and the ICC for the freezing behavior was 0.941 95% CI [0.912, 0.961]. The videos and all project information of DLC and SimBA used to obtain these values can be found on OSF.Data analysisStatistical analyses of behavior during tone presentation was preregistered for Experiments 2 and 3 (deviations from the preregistration are indicated below). Experiment 1 was not preregistered, but followed the analyses preregistered for Experiment 2. The 9 CSs were grouped in blocks of 3 CSs for acquisition and extinction phases. For the generalization phase a mean of the three tone presentations per session was calculated for the CS as well as for GS1 and GS2. Data were analyzed using (mixed) repeated-measures (RM) ANOVA and significant effects were followed up by post-hoc tests with multiple-test corrections. When assumptions were violated, nonparametric statistical analyses were performed. For the nonparametric mixed repeated measures ANOVAs, we applied an aligned rank transform ANOVA using the ARTool72 package from R (Version 4.1.3; R Core Team, 2022)73 and R Studio (Version 9.1.372; RStudio Team, 2021)74. All other statistical analyses were performed using afex75, rstatix76 or glmmTMB77. Data were processed and plotted using the tidyverse78, reshape79, and zoo80 packages.Experiment 1 and 2To analyze acquisition in Experiments 1 and 2, we used three repeated-measures ANOVAs on avoidance, freezing, and suppression of lever pressing during the first block of sessions 1, 2, and 10. Similar analyses were conducted to evaluate the generalization test, but we included stimulus type (CS, GS1, or GS2) as an additional within-subject measure. Therefore, generalization was assessed with three two-way (Day, Stimulus) repeated-measures ANOVAs on avoidance, freezing, and suppression of lever pressing. We analyzed extinction learning with three repeated measures ANOVAs comparing avoidance, freezing, and suppression of lever pressing from the first to the fourth session. These analyses were preregistered for Experiment 2, but not for Experiment 1.As secondary analyses, we preregistered for Experiment 2 the comparison of non-persistent and persistent avoider rats. For Experiment 1 we did the same analysis. A rat was considered a persistent avoider when the time on the platform during the CS was more than 50% during the first block of the first extinction day, a criterion similar to that used in a previous study17. We also preregistered that we would consider an animal to be a darter if it would exhibit at least one dart between the third and the seventh CS-US pairing during the first avoidance training session, based on41. We additionally preregistered testing sex differences between data to be collected in females (Experiment 2) and data previously collected in males (Experiment 1). We preregistered testing sex differences in avoidance, freezing, and suppression of bar pressing during avoidance training in sessions 1, 2, and 10 with 2 (Sex) × 3 (Session) mixed repeated-measures ANOVAs. We preregistered the same approach to study extinction, comparing sessions 1 to 4. For the generalization test, we added Stimulus type as a within-factor to the above mixed repeated-measures ANOVAs, comparing sessions 1 and 2. As an exploratory analysis, we also calculated a discrimination index for Experiments 1 and 2. Using RM ANOVAs we performed tests to compare the discrimination indices of avoidance and freezing between the three tone comparisons (CS vs GS1, CS vs GS2, and GS1 vs GS2) over sessions 1 and 2.Experiment 3We preregistered three mixed RM ANOVAs comparing the percentage of avoidance, freezing, and suppression between sexes in the first acquisition block in sessions 1, 2, and 10. We also preregistered a similar analysis for the generalization sessions, adding stimulus type (CS, GS1, or GS2) to the mixed ANOVA and using avoidance, freezing, discrimination index, and suppression of lever pressing as dependent variables. Finally, we preregistered testing sex differences in extinction learning with three mixed ANOVAs to compare avoidance, freezing, and suppression of lever pressing during the first to fourth extinction sessions.We preregistered the same secondary analyses to compare between non-persistent and persistent avoider rats as in Experiment 2. Finally, we investigated differences during extinction between distinct estrous phases. We classified the female rats as being into a high vs low estrogen phase and used a t-test to evaluate whether there were differences between the resulting groups in avoidance, freezing, or suppression of lever pressing.The classification of darters and non-darters was performed as preregistered and as reported by other authors previously41. We exploratorily investigated with a Chi-square whether females were more likely to be classified as darters than males. Additionally, we also explored whether there were differences in the number of dart bouts of darter rats during the three different phases of Experiment 3 (i.e., avoidance acquisition, generalization and extinction). Three negative binomial mixed-effect models were fitted, one per test phase, to predict number of darts (dependent variable) based on day and sex (predictors). These models included random intercepts for rats to account for individual variability.

Data availability

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available on the OSF repository: https://osf.io/ncb8j/.

ReferencesCraske, M. G. & Stein, M. B. Anxiety. Lancet 388, 3048–3059 (2016).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Baxter, A. J., Scott, K. M., Vos, T. & Whiteford, H. A. Global prevalence of anxiety disorders: a systematic review and meta-regression. Psychol. Med. 43, 897–910 (2013).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Beckers, T. et al. Understanding clinical fear and anxiety through the lens of human fear conditioning. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2, 233–245 (2023).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Lissek, S. et al. Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 1391–1424 (2005).PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Lonsdorf, T. B. et al. Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77, 247–285 (2017).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Arnaudova, I., Kindt, M., Fanselow, M. & Beckers, T. Pathways towards the proliferation of avoidance in anxiety and implications for treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 96, 3–13 (2017).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Craske, M. G., Hermans, D. & Vervliet, B. State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B Biol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0025 (2018).Article 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Dymond, S., Dunsmoor, J. E., Vervliet, B., Roche, B. & Hermans, D. Fear Generalization in Humans: Systematic Review and Implications for Anxiety Disorder Research. Behav. Ther. 46, 561–582 (2015).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Duits, P. et al. Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depress. Anxiety 32, 239–253 (2015).PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Pitman, R. K. & Orr, S. P. Test of the conditioning model of neurosis: Differential aversive conditioning of angry and neutral facial expressions in anxiety disorder patients. J. Abnormal Psychol. 95, 208–213 (1986).CAS 

Google Scholar 

Krypotos, A. M., Effting, M., Kindt, M. & Beckers, T. Avoidance learning: A review of theoretical models and recent developments. Front. Behav. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00189 (2015).Article 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Pittig, A., Boschet, J. M., Glück, V. M. & Schneider, K. Elevated costly avoidance in anxiety disorders: Patients show little downregulation of acquired avoidance in face of competing rewards for approach. Depress. Anxiety 38, 361–371 (2021).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Lissek, S. et al. Generalized Anxiety Disorder Is Associated With Overgeneralization of Classically Conditioned Fear. Biol. Psych. 75, 909–915 (2014).MATH 

Google Scholar 

Carpentier, N., Hermans, D. & Scheveneels, S. Predicting Emotional Distress, Based on Acquisition, Extinction, Avoidance, and Generalization Learning. Depress. Anxiety 2024, 6366269 (2024).PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Bravo-Rivera, C., Roman-Ortiz, C., Brignoni-Perez, E., Sotres-Bayon, F. & Quirk, G. J. Neural structures mediating expression and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance. J. Neurosci. 34, 9736–9742 (2014).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Diehl, M. M., Bravo-Rivera, C. & Quirk, G. J. The study of active avoidance: A platform for discussion. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 107, 229–237 (2019).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Bravo-Rivera, C., Roman-Ortiz, C., Montesinos-Cartagena, M. & Quirk, G. J. Persistent active avoidance correlates with activity in prelimbic cortex and ventral striatum. Front. Behav. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00184 (2015).Article 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Landin, J. D. & Chandler, L. J. Adolescent alcohol exposure alters threat avoidance in adulthood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 1098343. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1098343 (2023).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Martínez-Rivera, F. J., Bravo-Rivera, C., Velázquez-Díaz, C. D., Montesinos-Cartagena, M. & Quirk, G. J. Prefrontal circuits signaling active avoidance retrieval and extinction. Psychopharmacology 236, 399–406 (2019).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Rosas-Vidal, L. E. et al. Alteration of BDNF in the medial prefrontal cortex and the ventral hippocampus impairs extinction of avoidance. Neuropsychopharmacol 43, 2636–2644 (2018).CAS 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Velazquez-Hernandez, G. & Sotres-Bayon, F. Lateral Habenula Mediates Defensive Responses Only When Threat and Safety Memories Are in Conflict. Eneuro 8, 89 https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0482-20.2021 (2021).

Google Scholar 

López-Moraga, A., Luyten, L. & Beckers, T. A history of avoidance does not impact extinction learning in male rats. NPJ Sci. Learn. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-024-00223-z (2024).Article 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Vincent, C. J. et al. Sex divergent behavioral responses in platform-mediated avoidance and glucocorticoid receptor blockade. Psychoneuroendocrinology 159, 106417 (2024).

Google Scholar 

Martínez-Rivera, F. J. et al. Prolonged avoidance training exacerbates OCD-like behaviors in a rodent model. Transl. Psychiatry 10, 1–12 (2020).MATH 

Google Scholar 

Rodriguez-Romaguera, J., Greenberg, B. D., Rasmussen, S. A. & Quirk, G. J. An Avoidance-Based Rodent Model of Exposure With Response Prevention Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 534–540 (2016).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Beery, A. K. & Zucker, I. Sex bias in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 565–572 (2011).PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Becker, J. B. et al. Strategies and methods for research on sex differences in brain and behavior. Endocrinology 146, 1650–1673 (2005).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Shansky, R. M. Animal studies in both sexes. Science 364, 825–826 (2019).ADS 

CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Shansky, R. M. & Murphy, A. Z. Considering sex as a biological variable will require a global shift in science culture. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 457–464 (2021).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Zucker, I. & Beery, A. K. Males still dominate animal studies. Nature 465, 690 (2010).ADS 

CAS 

PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Woitowich, N. C., Beery, A. & Woodruff, T. A 10-year follow-up study of sex inclusion in the biological sciences. eLife 9, e56344 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56344 (2020).Hayes, S. N. & Redberg, R. F. Dispelling the Myths: Calling for Sex-Specific Reporting of Trial Results. Mayo Clinic Proc. 83, 523–525 (2008).

Google Scholar 

Correa-de-Araujo, R. Serious Gaps: How the Lack of Sex/Gender-Based Research Impairs Health. J. Women’s Health 15, 1116–1122 (2006).MATH 

Google Scholar 

Bravo-Rivera, H. et al. Characterizing Different Strategies for Resolving Approach-Avoidance Conflict. Front. Neurosci. 15, 608922 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.608922 (2021).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Maren, S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 897–931 (2001).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Daviu, N., Andero, R., Armario, A. & Nadal, R. Sex differences in the behavioural and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal response to contextual fear conditioning in rats. Hormones Behav. 66, 713–723 (2014).CAS 

Google Scholar 

Shanazz, K., Dixon-Melvin, R., Nalloor, R., Thumar, R. & Vazdarjanova, A. I. Sex Differences In Avoidance Extinction After Contextual Fear Conditioning: Anxioescapic Behavior In Female Rats. Neuroscience 497, 146–156 (2022).CAS 

PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Lynch, J., Cullen, P. K., Jasnow, A. M. & Riccio, D. C. Sex differences in the generalization of fear as a function of retention intervals. Learn. Mem. 20, 628–632 (2013).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Beatty, W. W., Beatty, P. A. & Bowman, R. E. A sex difference in the extinction of avoidance behavior in rats. Psychon. Sci. 23, 213–214 (1971).MATH 

Google Scholar 

van Haaren, F., van Hest, A. & Heinsbroek, R. P. W. Behavioral differences between male and female rats: Effects of gonadal hormones on learning and memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 14, 23–33 (1990).PubMed 

Google Scholar 

Gruene, T. M., Flick, K., Stefano, A., Shea, S. D. & Shansky, R. M. Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats. Elife 4, e11352  https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11352.001 (2015).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Schwacha, M. G., Wang, P. & Chaudry, I. H. 30 - Trauma Models for Studying the Influence of Gender and Aging. in Surgical Research (eds. Souba, W. W. & Wilmore, D. W.) 357–366 (Academic Press, San Diego, 2001). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012655330-7/50032-0.Faraji, J. et al. Sex-specific stress and biobehavioral responses to human experimenters in rats. Front. Neurosci. 16, 965500 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.965500 (2022).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Georgiou, P. et al. Experimenters’ sex modulates mouse behaviors and neural responses to ketamine via corticotropin releasing factor. Nat Neurosci 25, 1191–1200 (2022).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Pellman, B. A., Schuessler, B. P., Tellakat, M. & Kim, J. J. Sexually Dimorphic Risk Mitigation Strategies in Rats. eNeuro 4, ENEURO.0288–16.2017 https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0288-16.2017 (2017). Denti, A. & Epstein, A. Sex differences in the acquisition of two kinds of avoidance behavior in rats. Physiol. Behavior 8, 611–615 (1972).CAS 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Day, H. L. L., Reed, M. M. & Stevenson, C. W. Sex differences in discriminating between cues predicting threat and safety. Neurobiol. Learni. Memory 133, 196–203 (2016).MATH 

Google Scholar 

Trask, S., Reis, D. S., Ferrara, N. C. & Helmstetter, F. J. Decreased cued fear discrimination learning in female rats as a function of estrous phase. Learn Mem. 27, 254–257 (2020).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Chang, Y.-J. et al. Estrogen modulates sexually dimorphic contextual fear extinction in rats through estrogen receptor β. Hippocampus 19, 1142–1150 (2009).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Hwang, M. J. et al. Contribution of estradiol levels and hormonal contraceptives to sex differences within the fear network during fear conditioning and extinction. BMC Psychiatry 15, 295 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0673-9 (2015). PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Fenton, G. E., Halliday, D. M., Mason, R., Bredy, T. W. & Stevenson, C. W. Sex differences in learned fear expression and extinction involve altered gamma oscillations in medial prefrontal cortex. Neurobiol. Learn. Memory 135, 66–72 (2016).

Google Scholar 

Matsuda, S. et al. Sex differences in fear extinction and involvements of extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK). Neurobiol. Learn. Memory 123, 117–124 (2015).CAS 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Florido, A. et al. Sex differences in neural projections of fear memory processing in mice and humans. Science Advances 10, eadk3365 (2024).Velasco, E. R., Florido, A., Milad, M. R. & Andero, R. Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 103, 81–108 (2019).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Binette, A. N., Totty, M. S. & Maren, S. Sex differences in the immediate extinction deficit and renewal of extinguished fear in rats. PLOS ONE 17, e0264797 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264797 (2022).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Wen, Z., Fried, J., Pace-Schott, E. F., Lazar, S. W. & Milad, M. R. Revisiting sex differences in the acquisition and extinction of threat conditioning in humans. Learn. Mem. 29, 274–282 (2022).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Mitchell, J. R. et al. Darting across space and time: parametric modulators of sex-biased conditioned fear responses. Learn Mem 29, 171–180 (2022).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Trott, J. M., Hoffman, A. N., Zhuravka, I. & Fanselow, M. S. Conditional and unconditional components of aversively motivated freezing, flight and darting in mice. eLife 11, e75663 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.75663 (2022).Totty, M. S. et al. Behavioral and brain mechanisms mediating conditioned flight behavior in rats. Sci. Rep. 11, 8215 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-87559-3 (2021).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Chanthongdee, K. et al. Comprehensive ethological analysis of fear expression in rats using DeepLabCut and SimBA machine learning model. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 1440601 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1440601 (2024).PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Luyten, L., Schroyens, N., Hermans, D. & Beckers, T. Parameter optimization for automated behavior assessment: plug-and-play or trial-and-error?. Front. Behav. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00028 (2014).Article 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Goodwin, N. L. et al. Simple Behavioral Analysis (SimBA) as a platform for explainable machine learning in behavioral neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 27, 1411–1424 (2024).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Percie du Sert, N. et al. The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol 18, e3000410 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000410(2020). Diehl, M. M. et al. Active avoidance requires inhibitory signaling in the rodent prelimbic prefrontal cortex. eLife 7, e34657 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.34657 (2018).Marcondes, F. K., Bianchi, F. J. & Tanno, A. P. Determination of the estrous cycle phases of rats: Some helpful considerations. Brazilian J. Biol. 62, 609–614 (2002).CAS 

Google Scholar 

Xu, W. & Südhof, T. C. A neural circuit for memory specificity and generalization. Science 339, 1290–1295 (2013).ADS 

CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Quirk, G. J., Russo, G. K., Barron, J. L. & Lebron, K. The Role of Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in the Recovery of Extinguished Fear. J. Neurosci. 20, 6225–6231 (2000).CAS 

PubMed 

PubMed Central 

Google Scholar 

Mathis, A. et al. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1281–1289 (2018).CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Nath, T. et al. Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors. Nat. Protoc. 14, 2152–2176 (2019).ADS 

CAS 

PubMed 

MATH 

Google Scholar 

Insafutdinov, E., Pishchulin, L., Andres, B., Andriluka, M. & Schiele, B. DeeperCut: A Deeper, Stronger, and Faster Multi-person Pose Estimation Model. in Computer Vision–ECCV 2016: 14th European Conference, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, October 11–14, 2016, Proceedings, Part VI 14 (eds. Leibe, B., Matas, J., Sebe, N. & Welling, M.) 34–50 (Springer International Publishing, Cham, 2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46466-4_3.He, K., Zhang, X., Ren, S. & Sun, J. Deep Residual Learning for Image Recognition. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1512.03385 (2015).Elkin, L. A., Kay, M., Higgins, J. J. & Wobbrock, J. O. An Aligned Rank Transform Procedure for Multifactor Contrast Tests. in The 34th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology 754–768 (ACM, Virtual Event USA, 2021). https://doi.org/10.1145/3472749.3474784.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ (2022).RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. http://www.rstudio.com/ (2021).Singmann, H. et al. afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments. https://doi.org/10.32614/CRAN.package.afex (2023). Kassambara, A. rstatix: Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests. https://doi.org/10.32614/CRAN.package.rstatix (2021). Brooks, M. et al. glmmTMB: Generalized Linear Mixed Models using Template Model Builder. https://doi.org/10.32614/CRAN.package.glmmTMB (2024).Wickham, H. & RStudio. tidyverse: Easily Install and Load the ‘Tidyverse’. https://doi.org/10.32614/CRAN.package.tidyverse(2022). Wickham, H. Reshaping data with the reshape package. Journal of Statistical Software 21, (2007).Zeileis, A. & Grothendieck, G. zoo: S3 infrastructure for regular and irregular time series. J. Statist. Softw. 14, 1–27 (2005).MATH 

Google Scholar 

Download referencesAcknowledgementsWe thank Nathan De Henau and Jialing Ding for assistance with data collection and behavioral scoring. We thank Celine Fatouh, Samantha Piers, Louca Bah D’Oliveira and Adrià Túnez Aquilué for assistance with behavioral scoring. We thank Mathijs Franssen for assistance with programming. Graphical representations in the figures were created with BioRender.com.FundingThis work was supported by KU Leuven Research Grant 3H190245 and FWO PhD fellowship 11K3821N.Author informationAuthor notesLaura Luyten and Tom Beckers jointly supervised to this work.Authors and AffiliationsCentre for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, BelgiumAlba López-Moraga, Laura Luyten & Tom BeckersLeuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, BelgiumAlba López-Moraga, Laura Luyten & Tom BeckersAuthorsAlba López-MoragaView author publicationsYou can also search for this author inPubMed Google ScholarLaura LuytenView author publicationsYou can also search for this author inPubMed Google ScholarTom BeckersView author publicationsYou can also search for this author inPubMed Google ScholarContributionsA.L.-M., L.L. and T.B. conceived and designed the study. A.L.-M. collected data and performed the data analysis. A.L.-M. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. L.L. and T.B. edited and critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors reviewed the results and approved the final version of the manuscript. L.L. and T.B. are co-last authors.Corresponding authorsCorrespondence to

Laura Luyten or Tom Beckers.Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval

All experiments were performed in accordance with Belgian and European laws (Belgian Royal Decree of 29/05/2013 and European Directive 2010/63/EU) and the ARRIVE 2.063 guidelines and approved by the KU Leuven animal ethics committee (project license number: 011/2019).

Additional informationPublisher’s noteSpringer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.Supplementary InformationSupplementary Information.Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissionsAbout this articleCite this articleLópez-Moraga, A., Luyten, L. & Beckers, T. Generalization and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance in male and female rats.

Sci Rep 15, 9730 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-94265-xDownload citationReceived: 14 November 2024Accepted: 12 March 2025Published: 21 March 2025DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-94265-xShare this articleAnyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:Get shareable linkSorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.Copy to clipboard

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

KeywordsGeneralizationExtinctionPlatform-mediated AvoidanceFearAnxietyRat

Read full news in source page